File size: 62,375 Bytes
6fa4bc9
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
{
    "paper_id": "W09-0202",
    "header": {
        "generated_with": "S2ORC 1.0.0",
        "date_generated": "2023-01-19T06:39:48.262230Z"
    },
    "title": "Word Space Models of Lexical Variation",
    "authors": [
        {
            "first": "Yves",
            "middle": [],
            "last": "Peirsman",
            "suffix": "",
            "affiliation": {
                "laboratory": "",
                "institution": "University of Leuven Leuven",
                "location": {
                    "country": "Belgium"
                }
            },
            "email": "[email protected]"
        },
        {
            "first": "Dirk",
            "middle": [],
            "last": "Speelman",
            "suffix": "",
            "affiliation": {
                "laboratory": "",
                "institution": "University of Leuven",
                "location": {
                    "settlement": "Leuven",
                    "country": "Belgium"
                }
            },
            "email": "[email protected]"
        }
    ],
    "year": "",
    "venue": null,
    "identifiers": {},
    "abstract": "In the recognition of words that are typical of a specific language variety, the classic keyword approach performs rather poorly. We show how this keyword analysis can be complemented with a word space model constructed on the basis of two corpora: one representative of the language variety under investigation, and a reference corpus. This combined approach is able to recognize the markers of a language variety as words that not only have a significantly higher frequency as compared to the reference corpus, but also a different distribution. The application of word space models moreover makes it possible to automatically discover the lexical alternative to a specific marker in the reference corpus.",
    "pdf_parse": {
        "paper_id": "W09-0202",
        "_pdf_hash": "",
        "abstract": [
            {
                "text": "In the recognition of words that are typical of a specific language variety, the classic keyword approach performs rather poorly. We show how this keyword analysis can be complemented with a word space model constructed on the basis of two corpora: one representative of the language variety under investigation, and a reference corpus. This combined approach is able to recognize the markers of a language variety as words that not only have a significantly higher frequency as compared to the reference corpus, but also a different distribution. The application of word space models moreover makes it possible to automatically discover the lexical alternative to a specific marker in the reference corpus.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Abstract",
                "sec_num": null
            }
        ],
        "body_text": [
            {
                "text": "Different varieties of the same language often come with their lexical peculiarities. Some words may be restricted to a specific register, while other ones may have different meanings in different regions. In corpus linguistics, the most straightforward way of finding such words that are typical of one language variety is to compile a corpus of that variety and compare it to a reference corpus of another variety. The most obvious comparison takes on the form of a keyword analysis, which looks for the words that are significantly more frequent in the one corpus as compared to the other (Dunning, 1993; Scott, 1997; Rayson et al., 2004) . For the purposes of a language-variational study, this classic keyword approach often does not suffice, however. As Kilgarriff has argued, keyword statistics are far too sensitive to high frequencies or topical differences to be used in the study of vocabulary differences (Kilgarriff, 2001) . We there-fore put forward an approach that combines keyword statistics with distributional models of lexical semantics, or word space models (Sahlgren, 2006; Bullinaria and Levy, 2007; Pad\u00f3 and Lapata, 2007; Peirsman, 2008) . In this way, we not only check whether two words have significantly different frequencies in the two relevant language varieties, but also to what degree their distribution varies between the corpora.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 592,
                        "end": 607,
                        "text": "(Dunning, 1993;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF2"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 608,
                        "end": 620,
                        "text": "Scott, 1997;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF12"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 621,
                        "end": 641,
                        "text": "Rayson et al., 2004)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF9"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 917,
                        "end": 935,
                        "text": "(Kilgarriff, 2001)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF4"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1079,
                        "end": 1095,
                        "text": "(Sahlgren, 2006;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF10"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1096,
                        "end": 1122,
                        "text": "Bullinaria and Levy, 2007;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF0"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1123,
                        "end": 1145,
                        "text": "Pad\u00f3 and Lapata, 2007;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF6"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1146,
                        "end": 1161,
                        "text": "Peirsman, 2008)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF7"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Introduction",
                "sec_num": "1"
            },
            {
                "text": "In this paper, we will focus on the lexical differences between two regional varieties of Dutch. Dutch is interesting because it is the official language of two neighbouring countries, Belgium and the Netherlands. Between these two countries, there exists a considerable amount of lexical variation (Speelman et al., 2006) . There are words much more frequently used in one of the two varieties as well as terms that have a different meaning in the two regions. We will call such words markers of a specific lect -a general term for regiolects, dialects, or other language varieties that are specific to a certain region, genre, etc. By constructing a word space model on the basis of two corpora instead of one, we will show how the distributional approach to lexical semantics can aid the recognition of such lectal variation.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 299,
                        "end": 322,
                        "text": "(Speelman et al., 2006)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF13"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Introduction",
                "sec_num": "1"
            },
            {
                "text": "In the next section, we will point out the weaknesses of the classic keyword approach, and show how word space models can provide a solution. In section 3, we will discuss how our approach recognizes markers of a given lect. In section 4, we will demonstrate how it can automatically find the alternatives in the other language variety. Section 5 wraps up with conclusions and an outlook for future research.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Introduction",
                "sec_num": "1"
            },
            {
                "text": "Intuitively, the most obvious way of looking for words that mark a particular language variety, is to take a corpus that represents this variety, and calculate its keywords with respect to a reference \u03c7 2 log-likelihood keyword \u03c7 2 keyword log-likelihood frank/noun ('franc') 262492.0 frank/noun ('franc') 335587.3 meer/adj ('more') 149505.0 meer/adj ('more') 153811.6 foto/noun ('photograph') 84286.7 Vlaams/adj ('Flemish') 93723.2 Vlaams/adj ('Flemish') 83663.0 foto/noun ('photograph') 87235.1 veel/adj ('much'/'many') 73655.5 vrijdag/noun ('Friday') 77865.5 Belgisch/adj ('Belgian') 62280.2 veel/adj ('much'/'many') 74167.1 vrijdag/noun ('Friday') 59135.9 Belgisch/adj ('Belgian') 64786.0 toekomst/noun ('future') 42440.5 toekomst/noun ('future') 55879.1 dossier/noun ('file') 34623.3 dossier/noun ('file') 45570.0 Antwerps/adj ('Antwerp') 33659.1 ziekenhuis/noun ('hospital') 44093.3 Table 1 : Top 10 keywords for the Belgian newspaper corpus, as compared to the Twente Nieuws Corpus.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 266,
                        "end": 275,
                        "text": "('franc')",
                        "ref_id": null
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 324,
                        "end": 332,
                        "text": "('more')",
                        "ref_id": null
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 379,
                        "end": 393,
                        "text": "('photograph')",
                        "ref_id": null
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 444,
                        "end": 455,
                        "text": "('Flemish')",
                        "ref_id": null
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 575,
                        "end": 586,
                        "text": "('Belgian')",
                        "ref_id": null
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 641,
                        "end": 651,
                        "text": "('Friday')",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 889,
                        "end": 896,
                        "text": "Table 1",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2"
            },
            {
                "text": "corpus (Dunning, 1993; Scott, 1997; Rayson et al., 2004) . This keyword approach has two important weaknesses, however. First, it has been shown that statistically significant differences in the relative frequencies of a word may arise from high absolute frequencies rather than real lexical variation (Kilgarriff, 2001) . Second, in the explicit comparison of two language varieties, the keyword approach offers no way of telling what word in the reference corpus, if any, serves as the alternative to an identified marker. Word space models offer a solution to both of these problems. We will present this solution on the basis of two corpora of Dutch. The first is the Twente Nieuws Corpus (TwNC), a 300 million word corpus of Netherlandic Dutch newspaper articles from between 1999 and 2002. The second is a corpus of Belgian Dutch we compiled ourselves, with the goal of making it as comparable to the Twente Nieuws Corpus as possible. With newspaper articles from six major Belgian newspapers from the years 1999 to 2005, it totals over 1 billion word tokens. Here we will work with a subset of this corpus of around 200 million word tokens.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 7,
                        "end": 22,
                        "text": "(Dunning, 1993;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF2"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 23,
                        "end": 35,
                        "text": "Scott, 1997;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF12"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 36,
                        "end": 56,
                        "text": "Rayson et al., 2004)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF9"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 302,
                        "end": 320,
                        "text": "(Kilgarriff, 2001)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF4"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2"
            },
            {
                "text": "As our starting point, we calculated the keywords of the Belgian corpus with respect to the Netherlandic corpus, both on the basis of a chi-square test (with Yates' continuity correction) (Scott, 1997) and the log-likelihood ratio (Dunning, 1993) . We considered only words with a total frequency of at least 200 that moreover occurred at least five times in each of the five newspapers that make up the Belgian corpus. This last restriction was imposed in order to exclude idiosyncratic language use in any of those newspapers. The top ten resulting keywords, listed in Table 1 , show an overlap of 90% between the tests. The words fall into a number of distinct groups. Frank, Vlaams, Belgisch and Antwerps (this last word appears only in the \u03c7 2 top ten) indeed typically occur in Belgian Dutch, simply because they are so tightly connected with Belgian culture. Dossier may reflect a Belgian preference for this French loanword. Why the words meer, veel, vrijdag, toekomst and ziekenhuis (only in the log-likelihood top ten) are in the lists, however, is harder to explain. There does not appear to be a linguistically significant difference in use between the two language varieties, neither in frequency nor in sense. The presence of foto, finally, may reflect certain publishing habits of Belgian newspapers, but again, there is no obvious difference in use between Belgium and the Netherlands. In sum, these Belgian keywords illustrate the weakness of this approach in the modelling of lexical differences between two language varieties. This problem was already noted by Kilgarriff (2001) , who argues that \"[t]he LOB-Brown differences cannot in general be interpreted as British-American differences\". One of the reasons is that \"for very common words, high \u03c7 2 values are associated with the sheer quantity of evidence and are not necessarily associated with a pre-theoretical notion of distinctiveness\". One way to solve this issue is presented by Speelman et al. (2008) . In their so-called stable lexical markers analysis, the word frequencies in one corpus are compared to those in several reference corpora. The keyness of a word then corresponds to the number of times it appears in the resulting keyword lists of the first corpus. This repetitive test helps filter out spurious keywords whose statistical significance does not reflect a linguistically significant difference in frequency. Here we explore an alternative solution, which scores candidate markers on the basis of their contextual distribution in the two corpora, in a so-called bilectal word space.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 188,
                        "end": 201,
                        "text": "(Scott, 1997)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF12"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 231,
                        "end": 246,
                        "text": "(Dunning, 1993)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF2"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1580,
                        "end": 1597,
                        "text": "Kilgarriff (2001)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF4"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1960,
                        "end": 1982,
                        "text": "Speelman et al. (2008)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF14"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 571,
                        "end": 578,
                        "text": "Table 1",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Keywords",
                "sec_num": "2.1"
            },
            {
                "text": "Word space models (Sahlgren, 2006; Bullinaria and Levy, 2007; Pad\u00f3 and Lapata, 2007; Peirsman, 2008) capture the semantic similarity between two words on the basis of their distribution in a corpus. In these models, two words are similar when they often occur with the same context words, or when they tend to appear in the same syntactic relationships. For our purposes, we need to build a word space on the basis of two corpora, more or less in the vein of Rapp's (1999) method for the identification of translation equivalents. The main difference is that we use two corpora of the same language, each of which should be representative of one of the language varieties under investigation. All other variables should be kept as constant as possible, so that we can attribute differences in word use between the two corpora to lexical differences between the two lects. Next, we select the words that occur in both corpora (or a subset of the n most frequent words to reduce dimensionality) as the dimensions of the word space model. For each target word, we then build two context vectors, one for each corpus. These context vectors contain information about the distribution of the target word. We finally calculate the similarity between two context vectors as the cosine of the angle between them.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 18,
                        "end": 34,
                        "text": "(Sahlgren, 2006;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF10"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 35,
                        "end": 61,
                        "text": "Bullinaria and Levy, 2007;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF0"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 62,
                        "end": 84,
                        "text": "Pad\u00f3 and Lapata, 2007;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF6"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 85,
                        "end": 100,
                        "text": "Peirsman, 2008)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF7"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 459,
                        "end": 472,
                        "text": "Rapp's (1999)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF8"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2.2"
            },
            {
                "text": "One crucial parameter in the construction of word space models is their definition of distribution. Some models consider the syntactic relationships in which a target word takes part (Pad\u00f3 and Lapata, 2007) , while other approaches look at the collocation strength between a target and all of the words that occur within n words to its left and right (Bullinaria and Levy, 2007) . With these last word-based approaches, it has been shown that small context sizes in particular lead to good models of the semantic similarity between two words (Bullinaria and Levy, 2007; Peirsman, 2008) . So far, we have therefore performed experiments with context sizes of one, two and three words to the left and right of the target. These all gave very similar results. Experiments with other context sizes and with syntactic features will be carried out in the near future. In this paper, we report on the results of a word-based model with context size three.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 183,
                        "end": 206,
                        "text": "(Pad\u00f3 and Lapata, 2007)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF6"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 351,
                        "end": 378,
                        "text": "(Bullinaria and Levy, 2007)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF0"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 542,
                        "end": 569,
                        "text": "(Bullinaria and Levy, 2007;",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF0"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 570,
                        "end": 585,
                        "text": "Peirsman, 2008)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF7"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2.2"
            },
            {
                "text": "In order to identify the markers of Belgian Dutch, we start from the keyword lists above. For each of the keywords, we get their context vector from the Belgian corpus, and find the 100 most similar context vectors from the Netherlandic corpus. The words that correspond to these context vectors are called the 'nearest neighbours' to the keyword. In the construction of our word space model, we selected from both corpora the 4,000 most frequent words, and used the cross-section of 2,538 words as our set of dimensions or context features. The model then calculated the point-wise mutual information between the target and each of the 2,538 context words that occurred at least twice in its context. All words in the Netherlandic Dutch corpus with a frequency of at least 200, plus the target itself, were considered possible nearest neighbours to the target.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2.2"
            },
            {
                "text": "Generally, where there are no major differences in the use of a keyword between the two lects, it will have itself as its nearest neighbour. If this is not the case, this may identify the keyword as a marker of Belgian Dutch. For example, six words from the lists above have themselves as their nearest neighbour: meer, foto, veel, vrijdag, toekomst and ziekenhuis. These are indeed the keywords that made little sense from a language-variational perspective. Dossier is its own second nearest neighbour, which indicates that there is slightly less of a match between its Belgian and Netherlandic use. Finally, the words linked to Belgian culture -frank, Vlaams, Belgisch and Antwerps -are much lower in their own lists of nearest neighbours, or totally absent, which correctly identifies them as markers of Belgian Dutch. In short, the keyword analysis ensures that the word occurs much more frequently in Belgian Dutch than in Netherlandic Dutch; the word space approach checks if it also has a different distribution in the two corpora.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2.2"
            },
            {
                "text": "For markers of Belgian Dutch, we can interpret the nearest neighbour suggested by the system as the other variety's alternative to that marker. For instance, dossier has rapport as its nearest neighbour, a synonym which indeed has a high keyword value for our Netherlandic Dutch corpus. Similarly, the culture-related words have their Dutch equivalents as their distributionally most similar words: frank has gulden ('guilder'), Vlaams and Belgisch both have Nederlands ('Dutch'), and Antwerps has Amsterdams ('Amsterdam (adj.)'). This makes intuitive sense if we take meaning to be a relative concept, where for instance a concept like 'currency of this country' is instantiated by the franc in Belgium and the guilder in Holland -at least in the pre-Euro period. These findings suggest that our combined method can be applied more generally in order to automatically discover lexical differences between the two language varieties.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Bilectal Word Spaces",
                "sec_num": "2.2"
            },
            {
                "text": "First we want to investigate whether a bilectal word space model can indeed contribute to the correct identification of markers of Belgian Dutch on a larger scale. We therefore had both types of approaches -the simple keyword approach and the combined method -suggest a top 2,000 of possible markers on the basis of our two corpora. The combined approach uses the same word space method we described above, with 2,538 dimensions and a context size of three. Basing itself on the lists of nearest neighbours, it then reorders the list of keywords, so as to arrive at a ranking that reflects lectal variation better than the original one. To this goal, each keyword receives a new score, which is the multiplication of two individual numbers. The first number is its rank in the original keyword list. At this point we considered only the 5,000 highest scoring keywords. The second is based on a list that ranks the words according to their difference in distribution between the two corpora. Words that do not occur in their own list of 100 nearest neighbours appear at the top of the list (rank 1), followed by words that are their own 100th nearest neighbour (rank 2), and so on to the words that have themselves as nearest neighbour (rank 101). In the future we plan to consider different numbers of neighbours in order to punish words with very different distributions more or less heavily. At this stage, however, restricting the method to 100 nearest neighbours gives fine results. These two ranks are then multiplied to give a combined score, on the basis of which a final list of candidates for lectal variation is computed. The lower this combined score (reflecting either high keyword values, very different distributions in the two corpora, or both), the higher candidate marker evaluation frank/noun ('franc') culture Vlaams/adj ('Flemish') culture match/noun ('match') literature info/noun ('info') rijkswacht/noun ('state police') RBBN weekend/noun ('weekend') schepen/noun ('alderman') RBBN fr./noun ('franc') culture provinciaal/adj ('provincial') RBBN job/noun ('job') RBBN Table 2 : Top ten candidate markers suggested by the combined method on the basis of the loglikelihood ratio.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 2090,
                        "end": 2097,
                        "text": "Table 2",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "the likelihood that the word is a marker of Belgian Dutch. This approach thus ensures that words that have very different distributions in the two corpora are promoted with respect to the original keyword list, while words with very similar distributions are downgraded.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "As our Gold Standard we used the Reference List of Belgian Dutch (Referentiebestand Belgisch Nederlands, RBBN), a list of almost 4,000 words and expressions that are typical of Belgian Dutch (Martin, 2005) . These are classified into a number of groups -culturally-related terms (e.g., names of political parties), Belgian markers that are not lexicalized in Netherlandic Dutch, markers that are lexicalized in Netherlandic Dutch, etc. We used a subset of 717 one-word nouns, verbs and adjectives that appear at least 200 times in our Belgian corpus to evaluate our approach. Even if we informally explore the first ten candidate markers, the advantages of combining the loglikelihood ratio with the word space model already become clear (see table 2). Four of these candidates are in the RBBN gold standard. Similarly, frank, Vlaams and fr. are culturally related to Belgium, while match has been identified as a typically Belgian word in previous corpus-linguistic research (Geeraerts et al., 1999) . Info and weekend are not present in the external sources we consulted, but nevertheless show an interesting distribution with respect to their respective synonyms. In the Belgian corpus, info occurs more often than the longer and more formal information (32,009 vs 30,171), whereas in the Dutch corpus the latter is used about 25 times as frequently as the former (1,681 vs 41,429) . Similarly, the Belgian corpus contains far more instances of weekend than of its synonym weekeinde (35,406 vs 6,390) , while the Dutch corpus shows the reverse pattern (6,974 vs 28,234). These words are thus far better candidate markers than the original keywords meer, foto, veel, vrijdag, toekomst or ziekenhuis, which have disappeared from the top ten.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 191,
                        "end": 205,
                        "text": "(Martin, 2005)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF5"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 976,
                        "end": 1000,
                        "text": "(Geeraerts et al., 1999)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF3"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1367,
                        "end": 1384,
                        "text": "(1,681 vs 41,429)",
                        "ref_id": null
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1486,
                        "end": 1503,
                        "text": "(35,406 vs 6,390)",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "Let us now evaluate the methods more broadly, on the basis of the top 2,000 keywords they suggest. The left plot in Figure 1 shows their F-scores in function of the number of suggested markers; the right graph plots precision in function of recall. The two keyword approaches score rather similarly, with the log-likelihood ratio achieving slightly better results than the chi-square test. This superiority of the log-likelihood approach was already noted by Rayson et al. (2004) . Both combined methods give a very clear advantage over the simple keyword statistics, again with the best results for the log-likelihood ratio. For example, ten of the first 100 candidates suggested by both keyword approaches are present in our Gold Standard, giving a precision of 10% and a recall of 1.4% (Fscore 2.4%). Adding our word space model makes this figure rise to 29 correct markers, resulting in a precision of 29% and a recall of 4% (F-score 7.1%). This large advantage in performance is maintained further down the list. At 1000 candidates, the keyword approaches have a recall of around 20% (chi-square 19%, log-likelihood 21%) and a precision of around 14% (chi-square 14%, log-likelihood 15%). At the same point, the combined approaches have reached a recall of over 30% (chi-square 31%, log-likelihood 32%) with a precision of around 22% (chi-square 22%, loglikelihood 23%). Expressed differently, the best keyword approach needs around 500 candidates to recover 10% of the gold standard, 1000 to recover 20% and 2000 to recover 40%. This linear increase is outperformed by the best combined approach, which needs only 300, 600 and 1500 candidate markers to reach the same recall figures. This corresponds to relative gains of 40%, 40% and 25%. As these results indicate, the performance gain starts to diminish after 1000 candidates. Future experiments will help determine if this issue can be resolved with different parameter settings.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 459,
                        "end": 479,
                        "text": "Rayson et al. (2004)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF9"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 116,
                        "end": 124,
                        "text": "Figure 1",
                        "ref_id": "FIGREF1"
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "Despite these large gains in performance, the combined method still has problems with a number of Belgian markers. A manual analysis of these cases shows that they often have several senses, only one of which is typical of Belgian Dutch. The Reference List for instance contains fout ('mistake') and mossel ('mussel') as Belgian markers, with their specialized meanings 'foul (in sports)' and 'weakling'. Not only do these words have very low keyword values for the Belgian corpus; they also have very similar distributions in the two corpora, and are their own first and second neighbour, respectively. Sometimes a failure to recognize a particular marker is more due Table 3 : Manual analysis of the top 500 words suggested by the combined approach.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 669,
                        "end": 676,
                        "text": "Table 3",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "to the results of one individual method. This is for instance the case with the correct Belgian marker home ('(old people's) home').",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "Although the word space model does not find this word in its own list of nearest Netherlandic neighbours, it remains low on the marker list due to its fairly small log-likelihood ratio. Conversely, punt, graad and klaar are rather high on the keyword list of the Belgian corpus, but are downgraded, as they have themselves as their nearest neighbour. This is again because their status as a marker only applies to one infrequent meaning ('school mark', 'twoyear cycle of primary education' and 'clear') instead of the dominant meanings ('final stop, point (e.g., in sports)', 'degree' and 'ready'), which are shared between the two regional varieties. However, this last disadvantage applies to all markers that are much more frequently used in Belgium but still sometimes occur in the Netherlandic corpus with a similar distribution. Finally, because our Gold Standard is not an exhaustive list of Belgian Dutch markers, the results in Figure 1 are an underestimate of real performance. We therefore manually went through the top 500 markers suggested by the best combined approach and classified them into three new groups. The results of this analysis are presented in Table 3 . First, we consulted the Van Dale Groot Woordenboek der Nederlandse taal (Den Boon and Geeraerts, 2005) , the major dictionary of Dutch, which contains about 3,000 words marked with the label \"Belgian Dutch\". 11% of the first 100 and 9.4% of the first 500 candidates that were initially judged incorrect carry this label or have a definition that explicitly refers to Belgium. Second, we counted the words that are morphologically related to words in the Gold Standard or to Belgian words found in Van Dale. These are for instance compound nouns one of whose parts is present in the Gold Standard, which means that they are correct markers of Belgian Dutch as well. They represent 2% of the top 100 and 4.6% of the top 500. Third, we counted the words that are inherently linked to Belgian culture, mostly in the form of place names. This group corresponds to 25% of the first 100 and 12% of the first 500 candidate markers. This suggests that the true precision of our method at 100 and 500 candidates is thus at least 67% and 51.4%, respectively.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 1268,
                        "end": 1284,
                        "text": "Geeraerts, 2005)",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 937,
                        "end": 945,
                        "text": "Figure 1",
                        "ref_id": "FIGREF1"
                    },
                    {
                        "start": 1172,
                        "end": 1179,
                        "text": "Table 3",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Recognizing lectal differences",
                "sec_num": "3"
            },
            {
                "text": "The Reference List of Belgian Dutch not only lists Belgian Dutch words and expressions, but also gives their Netherlandic Dutch alternative, if one exists. Our word space model offers us a promising way of determining this alternative automatically, by looking at the nearest Netherlandic neighbours to a Belgian marker. As our Gold Standard, we selected from the Reference List those words with a frequency of at least 200 in the Belgian corpus whose Dutch alternative also had a frequency of at least 200 in the Dutch corpus. This resulted in a test set of 315 words: 240 nouns, 45 verbs and 30 adjectives. For each of these words, we used our word space model to find the 100 nearest Netherlandic neighbours, again with context size three but now with as dimensions all words shared between the two corpora, in order to improve performance. We then determined if the Dutch alternative was indeed in the list of nearest neighbours to the target. We started by looking at the single nearest neighbour only, and then step by step extended the list to include the 100 nearest neighbours. If a word had itself among its nearest neighbours, this neighbour was discarded and replaced by the next one down the list. The results are shown in Figure 2 . 11 out of 30 adjectives (36.7%), 10 out of 45 verbs (22.2%) and 56 out of 240 nouns (23.3%) had their Dutch alternative as their nearest neighbour. At ten nearest neighbours, these figures had risen to 60.0%, 48.9% and 44.6%. These encouraging results underpin the usefulness of word space models in languagevariational research. A manual analysis of Belgian markers for which the approach does not find the Netherlandic alternative again reveals that a large majority of these errors occur when polysemous words have only one, infrequent meaning that is typical of Belgian Dutch. For example, the dominant sense of the word tenor is obviously the 'male singer' meaning. In Belgium, however, this term can also refer to a leading figure, for instance in a political party or a sports discipline. Since this metaphorical sense is far less frequent than the literal one, the context vector fails to pick it up, and almost all nearest Netherlandic neighbours are related to opera or music. One way to solve this problem would be to abandon word space models that build only one context vector per word. Instead, we could cluster all individual contexts of a word, with the aim of identifying context clusters that correspond to the several senses of that word (Sch\u00fctze, 1998) . This is outside the scope of the current paper, however.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 2504,
                        "end": 2519,
                        "text": "(Sch\u00fctze, 1998)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF11"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 1236,
                        "end": 1244,
                        "text": "Figure 2",
                        "ref_id": "FIGREF2"
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Finding alternatives",
                "sec_num": "4"
            },
            {
                "text": "We have presented an application of word space models to language-variational research. To our knowledge, the construction of word space models on the basis of two corpora of the same language instead of one is new to both variational linguistics and Natural Language Processing. It complements the classic keyword approach in that it helps recognize those keywords that, in addition to their different relative frequencies in two language varieties, also have a substantially different distribution. An application of this method to Belgian Dutch showed that the keywords that pass this test indeed much more often represent markers of the language variety under investigation. Moreover, often the word space model also succeeded in identifying the Netherlandic Dutch alternative to the Belgian marker.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Conclusions and future research",
                "sec_num": "5"
            },
            {
                "text": "As the development of this approach is still in its early stages, we have committed ourselves more to its general presentation than to the precise parameter settings. In the near future, we therefore aim to investigate more fully the possible variation that the method allows. First, we will focus on the implementation of the word space model, by studying word-based models with other context sizes as well as syntax-based approaches. Second, we want to examine other ways in which the word-based model and the classic keyword approach can be combined, apart from the multiplication of ranks that we have proposed here. While this large freedom in parameter settings could be seen as a weakness of the proposed method, the fact that we obtained similar results for all settings we have tried out so far, adds to our confidence that word space models present a sensible complementation of the classic keyword approaches, irrespective of the precise parameter settings.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Conclusions and future research",
                "sec_num": "5"
            },
            {
                "text": "In addition to those modelling issues, there are a number of other extensions we would like to explore. First, the Gold Standard we have used so far is rather limited in scope. We therefore plan to incorporate more sources on language variation to test the robustness of our approach. Finally, as we have observed a number of times, the method in its present form is not sensitive to possibly infrequent meanings of a polysemous word. This may be solved by the application of a clustering approach that is able to cluster a word's contexts into several sense clusters (Sch\u00fctze, 1998) . Still, the promising results in this paper encourage us to believe that the current approach has a future as a new method in language-variational research and as a tool for lexicography.",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 568,
                        "end": 583,
                        "text": "(Sch\u00fctze, 1998)",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF11"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Conclusions and future research",
                "sec_num": "5"
            }
        ],
        "back_matter": [],
        "bib_entries": {
            "BIBREF0": {
                "ref_id": "b0",
                "title": "Extracting semantic representations from word cooccurrence statistics: A computational study",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "John",
                        "middle": [
                            "A"
                        ],
                        "last": "Bullinaria",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Joseph",
                        "middle": [
                            "P"
                        ],
                        "last": "Levy",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2007,
                "venue": "Behaviour Research Methods",
                "volume": "39",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "510--526",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "John A. Bullinaria and Joseph P. Levy. 2007. Ex- tracting semantic representations from word co- occurrence statistics: A computational study. Be- haviour Research Methods, 39:510-526.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF1": {
                "ref_id": "b1",
                "title": "Van Dale Groot Woordenboek van de Nederlandse taal",
                "authors": [],
                "year": 2005,
                "venue": "",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Ton Den Boon and Dirk Geeraerts. 2005. Van Dale Groot Woordenboek van de Nederlandse taal (14e ed.). Van Dale Lexicografie, Utrecht/Antwerp.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF2": {
                "ref_id": "b2",
                "title": "Accurate methods for the statistics of surprise and coincidence",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Ted",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Dunning",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1993,
                "venue": "Computational Linguistics",
                "volume": "19",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "61--74",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Ted Dunning. 1993. Accurate methods for the statis- tics of surprise and coincidence. Computational Linguistics, 19:61-74.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF3": {
                "ref_id": "b3",
                "title": "and Dirk Speelman",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Dirk",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Geeraerts",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Stefan",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Grondelaers",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1999,
                "venue": "Convergentie en Divergentie in de Nederlandse Woordenschat. Meertens Instituut",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Dirk Geeraerts, Stefan Grondelaers, and Dirk Speel- man. 1999. Convergentie en Divergentie in de Ned- erlandse Woordenschat. Meertens Instituut, Ams- terdam.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF4": {
                "ref_id": "b4",
                "title": "Comparing corpora",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Adam",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Kilgarriff",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2001,
                "venue": "International Journal of Corpus Linguistics",
                "volume": "6",
                "issue": "1",
                "pages": "1--37",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Adam Kilgarriff. 2001. Comparing corpora. Interna- tional Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 6(1):1-37.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF5": {
                "ref_id": "b5",
                "title": "Het Belgisch-Nederlands anders bekeken: het Referentiebestand Belgisch-Nederlands (RBBN)",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Willy",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Martin",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2005,
                "venue": "",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Willy Martin. 2005. Het Belgisch-Nederlands anders bekeken: het Referentiebestand Belgisch- Nederlands (RBBN). Technical report, Vrije Uni- versiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Holland.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF6": {
                "ref_id": "b6",
                "title": "Dependency-based construction of semantic space models",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Sebastian",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Pad\u00f3",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Mirella",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Lapata",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2007,
                "venue": "Computational Linguistics",
                "volume": "33",
                "issue": "2",
                "pages": "161--199",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Sebastian Pad\u00f3 and Mirella Lapata. 2007. Dependency-based construction of semantic space models. Computational Linguistics, 33(2):161-199.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF7": {
                "ref_id": "b7",
                "title": "Word space models of semantic similarity and relatedness",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Yves",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Peirsman",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2008,
                "venue": "Proceedings of the 13th ESSLLI Student Session",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "143--152",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Yves Peirsman. 2008. Word space models of seman- tic similarity and relatedness. In Proceedings of the 13th ESSLLI Student Session, pages 143-152.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF8": {
                "ref_id": "b8",
                "title": "Automatic identification of word translations from unrelated English and German corpora",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Reinhard",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Rapp",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1999,
                "venue": "Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL)",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "519--526",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Reinhard Rapp. 1999. Automatic identification of word translations from unrelated English and Ger- man corpora. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Lin- guistics (ACL), pages 519-526, College Park, Mary- land.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF9": {
                "ref_id": "b9",
                "title": "Extending the cochran rule for the comparison of word frequencies between corpora",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Paul",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Rayson",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Damon",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Berridge",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Brian",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Francis",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2004,
                "venue": "Proceedings of the 7i\u00e8mes Journ\u00e9es Internationales d'Analyse Statistique des Donn\u00e9es Textuelles (JADT 2004)",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "926--936",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Paul Rayson, Damon Berridge, and Brian Francis. 2004. Extending the cochran rule for the com- parison of word frequencies between corpora. In Proceedings of the 7i\u00e8mes Journ\u00e9es Internationales d'Analyse Statistique des Donn\u00e9es Textuelles (JADT 2004), pages 926-936, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF10": {
                "ref_id": "b10",
                "title": "Using Distributional Analysis to Represent Syntagmatic and Paradigmatic Relations Between Words in High-dimensional Vector Spaces",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Magnus",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Sahlgren",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2006,
                "venue": "",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Magnus Sahlgren. 2006. The Word-Space Model. Using Distributional Analysis to Represent Syntag- matic and Paradigmatic Relations Between Words in High-dimensional Vector Spaces. Ph.D. thesis, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF11": {
                "ref_id": "b11",
                "title": "Automatic word sense discrimination",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Hinrich",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Sch\u00fctze",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1998,
                "venue": "Computational Linguistics",
                "volume": "24",
                "issue": "1",
                "pages": "97--124",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Hinrich Sch\u00fctze. 1998. Automatic word sense dis- crimination. Computational Linguistics, 24(1):97- 124.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF12": {
                "ref_id": "b12",
                "title": "PC analysis of key words -and key key words",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Mike",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Scott",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1997,
                "venue": "System",
                "volume": "25",
                "issue": "2",
                "pages": "233--245",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Mike Scott. 1997. PC analysis of key words -and key key words. System, 25(2):233-245.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF13": {
                "ref_id": "b13",
                "title": "A profile-based calculation of region and register variation: The synchronic and diachronic status of the two main national varieties of Dutch",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Dirk",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Speelman",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Stefan",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Grondelaers",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Dirk",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Geeraerts",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2006,
                "venue": "Corpus Linguistics around the World",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "195--202",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Dirk Speelman, Stefan Grondelaers, and Dirk Geer- aerts. 2006. A profile-based calculation of re- gion and register variation: The synchronic and di- achronic status of the two main national varieties of Dutch. In Andrew Wilson, Dawn Archer, and Paul Rayson, editors, Corpus Linguistics around the World, pages 195-202. Rodopi, Amsterdam.",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF14": {
                "ref_id": "b14",
                "title": "Variation in the choice of adjectives in the two main national varieties of Dutch",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "Dirk",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Speelman",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Stefan",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Grondelaers",
                        "suffix": ""
                    },
                    {
                        "first": "Dirk",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Geeraerts",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 2008,
                "venue": "Gitte Kristiansen and Ren\u00e9 Dirven",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "205--233",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Dirk Speelman, Stefan Grondelaers, and Dirk Geer- aerts. 2008. Variation in the choice of adjectives in the two main national varieties of Dutch. In Gitte Kristiansen and Ren\u00e9 Dirven, editors, Cogni- tive Sociolinguistics. Language Variation, Cultural Models, Social Systems, pages 205-233. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin.",
                "links": null
            }
        },
        "ref_entries": {
            "FIGREF1": {
                "uris": null,
                "text": "Precision and recall figures of the keyword methods and the combined approaches.",
                "type_str": "figure",
                "num": null
            },
            "FIGREF2": {
                "uris": null,
                "text": "Percentage of markers of Belgian Dutch whose Netherlandic alternative is present among their n nearest neighbours.",
                "type_str": "figure",
                "num": null
            }
        }
    }
}