Benjamin Aw
Add updated pkl file v3
6fa4bc9
{
"paper_id": "W10-0206",
"header": {
"generated_with": "S2ORC 1.0.0",
"date_generated": "2023-01-19T05:07:12.858253Z"
},
"title": "A Text-driven Rule-based System for Emotion Cause Detection",
"authors": [
{
"first": "Sophia",
"middle": [],
"last": "Yat",
"suffix": "",
"affiliation": {},
"email": "[email protected]"
},
{
"first": "Mei",
"middle": [],
"last": "Lee",
"suffix": "",
"affiliation": {
"laboratory": "",
"institution": "The Hong Kong Polytechnic University",
"location": {}
},
"email": ""
},
{
"first": "Ying",
"middle": [],
"last": "Chen",
"suffix": "",
"affiliation": {
"laboratory": "",
"institution": "The Hong Kong Polytechnic University",
"location": {}
},
"email": "[email protected]"
},
{
"first": "Chu-Ren",
"middle": [],
"last": "Huang",
"suffix": "",
"affiliation": {
"laboratory": "",
"institution": "The Hong Kong Polytechnic University",
"location": {}
},
"email": "[email protected]"
}
],
"year": "",
"venue": null,
"identifiers": {},
"abstract": "Emotion cause detection is a new research area in emotion processing even though most theories of emotion treat recognition of a triggering cause event as an integral part of emotion. As a first step towards fully automatic inference of cause-emotion correlation, we propose a textdriven, rule-based approach to emotion cause detection in this paper. First of all, a Chinese emotion cause annotated corpus is constructed based on our proposed annotation scheme. By analyzing the corpus data, we identify seven groups of linguistic cues and generalize two sets of linguistic rules for detection of emotion causes. With the linguistic rules, we then develop a rule-based system for emotion cause detection. In addition, we propose an evaluation scheme with two phases for performance assessment. Experiments show that our system achieves a promising performance for cause occurrence detection as well as cause event detection. The current study should lay the ground for future research on the inferences of implicit information and the discovery of new information based on cause-event relation.",
"pdf_parse": {
"paper_id": "W10-0206",
"_pdf_hash": "",
"abstract": [
{
"text": "Emotion cause detection is a new research area in emotion processing even though most theories of emotion treat recognition of a triggering cause event as an integral part of emotion. As a first step towards fully automatic inference of cause-emotion correlation, we propose a textdriven, rule-based approach to emotion cause detection in this paper. First of all, a Chinese emotion cause annotated corpus is constructed based on our proposed annotation scheme. By analyzing the corpus data, we identify seven groups of linguistic cues and generalize two sets of linguistic rules for detection of emotion causes. With the linguistic rules, we then develop a rule-based system for emotion cause detection. In addition, we propose an evaluation scheme with two phases for performance assessment. Experiments show that our system achieves a promising performance for cause occurrence detection as well as cause event detection. The current study should lay the ground for future research on the inferences of implicit information and the discovery of new information based on cause-event relation.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Abstract",
"sec_num": null
}
],
"body_text": [
{
"text": "Text-based emotion processing has attracted plenty of attention in NLP. Most research has focused on the emotion detection and classification by identifying the emotion types, for instances happiness and sadness, for a given sentence or document (Alm 2005 , Mihalcea and Liu 2006 , Tokuhisa et al. 2008 . However, on top of this surface level information, deeper level information regarding emotions, such as the experiencer, cause, and result of an emotion, needs to be extracted and analyzed for real world applications (Alm 2009) .",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 246,
"end": 255,
"text": "(Alm 2005",
"ref_id": "BIBREF0"
},
{
"start": 256,
"end": 279,
"text": ", Mihalcea and Liu 2006",
"ref_id": "BIBREF14"
},
{
"start": 280,
"end": 302,
"text": ", Tokuhisa et al. 2008",
"ref_id": "BIBREF21"
},
{
"start": 522,
"end": 532,
"text": "(Alm 2009)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF1"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Introduction",
"sec_num": "1"
},
{
"text": "In this paper, we aim at mining one of the crucial deep level types of information, i.e. emotion cause, which provides useful information for applications ranging from economic forecasting, public opinion mining, to product design. Emotion cause detection is a new research area in emotion processing. In emotion processing, the cause event and emotion correlation is a fertile ground for extraction and entailment of new information. As a first step towards fully automatic inference of causeemotion correlation, we propose a text-driven, rule-based approach to emotion cause detection in this paper.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Introduction",
"sec_num": "1"
},
{
"text": "Most theories of emotion treat recognition of a triggering cause event as an integral part of emotional experience (Descartes 1649 , James 1884 , Plutchik 1962 , Wierzbicka 1999 . In this study, cause events refer to the explicitly expressed arguments or events that evoke the presence of the corresponding emotions. They are usually expressed by means of propositions, nominalizations, and nominals. For example, \"they like it\" is the cause event of the emotion happiness in the sentence \"I was very happy that they like it\". Note that we only take into account emotions that are explicitly expressed, which are usually presented by emotion keywords, e.g. \"This surprises me\". Implicit emotions that require inference or connotation are not processed in this first study. In this study, we first build a Chinese emotion cause annotated corpus with five primary emotions, i.e. happiness, sadness, anger, fear, and surprise. We then examine various linguistic cues which help detect emotion cause events: the position of cause event and experiencer relative to the emotion keyword, causative verbs (e.g. rang4 \"to cause\"), action verbs (e.g. xiang3dao4 \"to think about\"), epistemic markers (e.g. kan4jian4 \"to see\"), conjunctions (e.g. yin1wei4 \"because\"), and prepositions (e.g. dui4yu2 \"for\"). With the help of these cues, a list of linguistic rules is generalized. Based on the linguistic rules, we develop a rulebased system for emotion cause detection. Experiments show that such a rule-based system performs promisingly well. We believe that the current study should lay the ground for future research on inferences and discovery of new information based on cause-event relation, such as detection of implicit emotion or cause, as well as prediction of public opinion based on cause events, etc.",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 115,
"end": 130,
"text": "(Descartes 1649",
"ref_id": "BIBREF5"
},
{
"start": 131,
"end": 143,
"text": ", James 1884",
"ref_id": "BIBREF7"
},
{
"start": 144,
"end": 159,
"text": ", Plutchik 1962",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 160,
"end": 177,
"text": ", Wierzbicka 1999",
"ref_id": "BIBREF24"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Introduction",
"sec_num": "1"
},
{
"text": "The paper is organized as follows. Section 2 discusses the related work on various aspects of emotion analysis. Section 3 describes the construction of the emotion cause corpus. Section 4 presents our rule-based system for emotion cause detection. Section 5 describes its evaluation and performance. Section 6 highlights our main contributions.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Introduction",
"sec_num": "1"
},
{
"text": "We discuss previous studies on emotion analysis in this section, and underline fundamental yet unresolved issues. We survey the previous attempts on textual emotion processing and how the present study differs.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Previous Work",
"sec_num": "2"
},
{
"text": "Various approaches to emotion classification were proposed in different fields, such as philosophy (Spinoza 1675 , James 1884 , biology (Darwin 1859 , linguistics (Wierzbicka 1999 , K\u00f6vecses 2000 , neuropsychology (Plutchik 1962 , Turner 1996 , and computer science (Ortony et al. 1988 , Picard 1995 , as well as varying from language to language. Although there is lack of agreement among different theories on emotion classification, a small number of primary emotions are commonly assumed. Other emotions are secondary emotions which are the mixtures of the primary emotions.",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 99,
"end": 112,
"text": "(Spinoza 1675",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 113,
"end": 125,
"text": ", James 1884",
"ref_id": "BIBREF7"
},
{
"start": 136,
"end": 148,
"text": "(Darwin 1859",
"ref_id": "BIBREF4"
},
{
"start": 149,
"end": 179,
"text": ", linguistics (Wierzbicka 1999",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 180,
"end": 195,
"text": ", K\u00f6vecses 2000",
"ref_id": "BIBREF9"
},
{
"start": 214,
"end": 228,
"text": "(Plutchik 1962",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 229,
"end": 242,
"text": ", Turner 1996",
"ref_id": "BIBREF22"
},
{
"start": 266,
"end": 285,
"text": "(Ortony et al. 1988",
"ref_id": "BIBREF15"
},
{
"start": 286,
"end": 299,
"text": ", Picard 1995",
"ref_id": "BIBREF16"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Emotion Classes",
"sec_num": "2.1"
},
{
"text": "Researchers have attempted to propose the list of primary emotions, varying from two to ten basic emotions (Ekman 1984 , Plutchik 1980 , Turner 2000 . Fear and anger appear on every list, whereas happiness and sadness appear on most of the lists. These four emotions, i.e. fear, anger, happiness, and sadness, are the most common primary emotions. Other less common primary emotions are surprise, disgust, shame, distress, guilt, interest, pain, and acceptance. In this study, we adopt Turner's emotion classification (2000) , which identifies five primary emotions, namely happiness, sadness, fear, anger, and surprise. Turner's list consists of primary emotions agreed upon by most previous work.",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 107,
"end": 118,
"text": "(Ekman 1984",
"ref_id": "BIBREF6"
},
{
"start": 119,
"end": 134,
"text": ", Plutchik 1980",
"ref_id": "BIBREF17"
},
{
"start": 135,
"end": 148,
"text": ", Turner 2000",
"ref_id": "BIBREF23"
},
{
"start": 397,
"end": 461,
"text": "disgust, shame, distress, guilt, interest, pain, and acceptance.",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 518,
"end": 524,
"text": "(2000)",
"ref_id": null
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Emotion Classes",
"sec_num": "2.1"
},
{
"text": "Textual emotion processing is still in its early stages in NLP. Most of the previous works focus on emotion classification given a known emotion context such as a sentence or a document using either rule-based (Masum et al. 2007 , Chaumartin 2007 or statistical approaches (Mihalcea and Liu 2005, Kozareva et al. 2007) . However, the performance is far from satisfactory. What is more, many basic issues remain unresolved, for instances, the relationships among emotions, emotion type selection, etc. Tokuhisa et al. (2008) was the first to explore both the issues of emotion detection and classification. It created a Japanese emotion-provoking event corpus for an emotion classification task using an unsupervised approach. However, only 49.4% of cases were correctly labeled. Chen et al. (2009) developed two cognitive-based Chinese emotion corpora using a semi-unsupervised approach, i.e. an emotion-sentence (sentences containing emotions) corpus and a neutral-sentence (sentences containing no emotion) corpus. They showed that studies based on the emotion-sentence corpus (~70%) outperform previous corpora.",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 210,
"end": 228,
"text": "(Masum et al. 2007",
"ref_id": "BIBREF13"
},
{
"start": 229,
"end": 246,
"text": ", Chaumartin 2007",
"ref_id": "BIBREF3"
},
{
"start": 273,
"end": 286,
"text": "(Mihalcea and",
"ref_id": "BIBREF14"
},
{
"start": 287,
"end": 318,
"text": "Liu 2005, Kozareva et al. 2007)",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 501,
"end": 523,
"text": "Tokuhisa et al. (2008)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF21"
},
{
"start": 779,
"end": 797,
"text": "Chen et al. (2009)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF2"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Emotion Processing in Text",
"sec_num": "2.2"
},
{
"text": "Little research, if not none, has been done to examine the interactions between emotions and the corresponding cause events, which may make a great step towards an effective emotion classification model. The lack of research on cause events restricted current emotion analysis to simple classificatory work without exploring the potentials of the rich applications of putting emotion 'in context'. In fact, emotions can be invoked by perceptions of external events and in turn trigger reactions. The ability to detect implicit invoking causes as well as predict actual reactions will add rich dimensions to emotion analysis and lead to further research on event computing.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Emotion Processing in Text",
"sec_num": "2.2"
},
{
"text": "This section briefly describes how the emotion cause corpus is constructed. We first explain what an emotion cause is and discuss how emotion cause is linguistically expressed in Chinese. We then describe the corpus data and the annotation scheme. For more detailed discussion on the construction of the emotion cause corpus, please refer to Lee et al. (2010) .",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 342,
"end": 359,
"text": "Lee et al. (2010)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF11"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Emotion Cause Corpus",
"sec_num": "3"
},
{
"text": "Following Talmy (2000) , the cause of an emotion should be an event itself. In this work, it is called a cause event. By cause event, we do not necessarily mean the actual trigger of the emotion or what leads to the emotion. Rather, it refers to the immediate cause of the emotion, which can be the actual trigger event or the perception of the trigger event. Adapting TimeML annotation scheme (Saur\u00ed et al. 2004) , events refer to situations that happen or occur. In this study, cause events specifically refer to the explicitly expressed arguments or events that are highly linked with the presence of the corresponding emotions. In Lee et al.'s (2010) corpus, cause events are categorized into two types: verbal events and nominal events. Verbal events refer to events that involve verbs (i.e. propositions and nominalizations), whereas nominal events are simply nouns (i.e. nominals). Some examples of cause event types are given in bold face in (1)-(6).",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 10,
"end": 22,
"text": "Talmy (2000)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF20"
},
{
"start": 394,
"end": 413,
"text": "(Saur\u00ed et al. 2004)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF18"
},
{
"start": 635,
"end": 654,
"text": "Lee et al.'s (2010)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF11"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Cause Events",
"sec_num": "3.1"
},
{
"text": "(1) Zhe4-DET tou2-CL niu2-cattle de-POSS zhu3ren2-owner,",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Cause Events",
"sec_num": "3.1"
},
{
"text": "huo4-trouble lai2-come le-ASP, fei1chang2-very hai4pa4-frighten, jiu4-then ba3-PREP zhe4-DET tou2-CL niu2-cattle di1jia4-low price mai4chu1-sell.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "\"The owner was frightened to see that his cattle caused troubles, so he sold it at a low price.\"",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "(2) Mei2-not xiang3dao4-think ta1-3.SG.F shuo1-say de-POSS dou1-all shi4-is zhen1-true hua4-word, rang4lead ta1-3.SG.M zhen4jing1-shocked bu4yi3-very.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "\"He was shocked that what she said was the truth.\"",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "(3) Ta1 \"He was very happy about this loving idea.\"",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 4,
"end": 7,
"text": "Ta1",
"ref_id": "TABREF1"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "(4) Zhe4-DET ci4-CL yan3chu1-performance de-POSS jing1zhi4-exquisite dao4shi4-is ling4-cause wo3-1.SG shi2fen1-very jing1ya4-surprise.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "\"I was very surprised by this exquisite performance.\"",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "(5) Ni2ao4-Leo de-POSS hua4-word hen3-very ling4-make kai3luo4lin2-Caroline shang1xin1-sad.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "\"Caroline was very saddened by Leo's words.\"",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "(6) Dui4yu2-for wei4lai2-future, lao3shi2shuo1-frankly wo3-1.SG hen3-very hai4pa4-scared.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "\"Frankly, I am very scared about the future.\"",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "The causes in (1) and(2) are propositional causes, which indicate the actual events involved in causing the emotions. The ones in 3and 4are nominalized causes, whereas (5) and (6) involve nominal causes",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "yan3kan4-see zi4ji3-oneself de-POSS niu2-cattle re3chu1-cause",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "Based on the list of 91 Chinese primary emotion keywords identified in Chen et al. 2009, we extract 6,058 instances of sentences by keyword matching from the Sinica Corpus 1 , which is a tagged balanced corpus of Mandarin Chinese containing a total of ten million words. Each instance contains the focus sentence with the emotion keyword \"<FocusSentence>\" plus the sentence before \"<PrefixSentence>\" and after \"<SuffixSentence>\" it. The extracted instances include all primary emotion keywords occurring in the Sinica Corpus except for the emotion class happiness, as the keywords of happiness exceptionally outnumber other emotion classes. In order to balance the number of each emotion class, we set the upper limit at about 1,600 instances for each primary emotion.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Corpus Data and Annotation Scheme",
"sec_num": "3.2"
},
{
"text": "Note that the presence of emotion keywords does not necessarily convey emotional information due to different possible reasons such as negative polarity and sense ambiguity. Hence, by manual inspection, we remove instances that 1) are nonemotional; 2) contain highly ambiguous emotion keywords, such as ru2yi4 \"wish-fulfilled\", hai4xiu1 \"to be shy\", wei2nan2 \"to feel awkward\", from the corpus. After the removal, the remaining instances in the emotion cause corpus is 5,629. Among the remaining instances, we also remove the emotion keywords in which the instances do not express that particular emotion and yet are emotional. The total emotion keywords in the corpus is 5,958.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Corpus Data and Annotation Scheme",
"sec_num": "3.2"
},
{
"text": "For each emotional instance, two annotators manually annotate cause events of each keyword. Since more than one emotion can be present in an instance, the emotion keywords are tagged as <emotionword id=0>, <emotionword id=1>, and so on. Figure 1 shows an example of annotated emotional sentences in corpus, presented as pinyin with tones, followed by an English translation. For an emotion keyword tagged as <id=0>, [*01n] marks the beginning of its cause event while [*02n] marks the end. The \"0\" shows which index of emotion keyword it refers to, \"1\" marks the beginning of the cause event, \"2\" marks the end, and \"n\" indicates that the cause is a nominal event. For an emotion keyword tagged as <id=1>, [*11e] marks the beginning of the cause event while [*12e] marks the end, in which \"e\" refers to a verbal event, i.e. either a proposition or a nominalization. An emotion keyword can sometimes be associated with more than one cause, in which case both causes are marked. The emotional sentences containing no explicitly expressed cause event remain as they are.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 237,
"end": 245,
"text": "Figure 1",
"ref_id": "FIGREF1"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Corpus Data and Annotation Scheme",
"sec_num": "3.2"
},
{
"text": "The actual number of extracted instances of each emotion class to be analyzed, the positive emotional instances, and the instances with cause events are presented in Table 1 . We can see that 72% of the extracted instances express emotions, and 81% of the emotional instances have a cause. The corpus contains happiness (1,327) instances the most and sadness (616) the least. For each emotion type, about 81% of the emotional sentences, on average, are considered as containing a cause event, with surprise the highest (85%) and anger the lowest (73%). This indicates that an emotion mostly occurs with the cause event explicitly expressed in the text, which confirms the prominent role of cause events in expressing an emotion.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 166,
"end": 173,
"text": "Table 1",
"ref_id": "TABREF1"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Corpus Data and Annotation Scheme",
"sec_num": "3.2"
},
{
"text": "By analyzing the corpus data, we examine the correlations between emotions and cause events in terms of various linguistic cues: the position of cause event, verbs, epistemic markers, conjunctions, and prepositions. We hypothesize that these cues will facilitate the detection of emotion cause events.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "A Rule-based System for Cause Detection 4.1 Linguistic Analysis of Emotion Causes",
"sec_num": "4"
},
{
"text": "First, we calculate the distribution of cause event types of each emotion as well as the position of cause events relative to emotion keywords and experiencers. The total number of emotional instances regarding each emotion is given in Table 2 . Table 2 suggests that emotion cause events tend to be expressed by verbal events than nominal events and that cause events tend to occur at the position to the left of the emotion keyword, with fear (52%) being no preference. This may be attributed to the fact that fear can be triggered by either factive or potential causes, which is rare for other primary emotions. For fear, factive causes tend to take the left position whereas potential causes tend to take the right position.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 236,
"end": 244,
"text": "Table 2",
"ref_id": "TABREF2"
},
{
"start": 247,
"end": 254,
"text": "Table 2",
"ref_id": "TABREF2"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "A Rule-based System for Cause Detection 4.1 Linguistic Analysis of Emotion Causes",
"sec_num": "4"
},
{
"text": "Second, we identify seven groups of linguistic cues that are highly collocated with cause events (Lee et al. 2010) , as shown in Table 3 . Group I includes three common causative verbs, and Group II a list of verbs of thinking and talking. Group III is a list of say verbs. Group IV includes four types of epistemic markers which are usually verbs marking the cognitive awareness of emotion in the complement position (Lee and Huang 2009) . The epistemic markers include verbs of seeing, hearing, knowing, and existing. Group V covers some prepositions which all roughly mean 'for'. Group VI contains the conjunctions that explicitly mark the emotion cause. Group VII includes other linguistic cues that do not fall into any of the six groups. Each group of linguistic cues serves as an indicator marking the cause events in different structures of emotional constructions, in which Group I specifically marks the end of the cause events while the other six groups marks the beginning of the cause events.",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 97,
"end": 114,
"text": "(Lee et al. 2010)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF11"
},
{
"start": 418,
"end": 438,
"text": "(Lee and Huang 2009)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF12"
}
],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 129,
"end": 136,
"text": "Table 3",
"ref_id": "TABREF3"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "A Rule-based System for Cause Detection 4.1 Linguistic Analysis of Emotion Causes",
"sec_num": "4"
},
{
"text": "We examine 100 emotional sentences of each emotion keyword randomly extracted from the development data, and generalize some rules for identifying the cause of the corresponding emotion verb (Lee 2010) . The cause is considered as a proposition. It is generally assumed that a proposition has a verb which optionally takes a noun occurring before it as the subject and a noun after it as the object. However, a cause can also be expressed as a nominal. In other words, both the predicate and the two arguments are optional provided that at least one of them is present. We also manually identify the position of the experiencer as well as the linguistic cues discussed in Section 4.1. All these components may occur in the clause containing the emotion verb (focus clause), the clause before the focus clause, or the clause after the focus clause. The abbreviations used in the rules are given as follows: C = Cause event E = Experiencer K = Keyword/emotion verb B = Clause before the focus clause F = Focus clause/the clause containing the emotion verb A = Clause after the focus clause For illustration, an example of the rule description is given in Rule 1. Rule 1 indicates that the experiencer (E) appears to be the nearest Na (common noun)/ Nb (proper noun)/ Nc (place noun)/ Nh (pronoun) after Group I cue words in the focus clause (F), while, at the same time, it comes before the keyword (K). Besides, the cause (C) comes before Group I cue words. We simplify the proposition as a structure of (N)+(V)+(N), which is very likely to contain the cause event. Theoretically, in identifying C, we should first look for the nearest verb occurring before Group I cue words in the focus sentence (F) or the clause before the focus clause (B), and consider this verb as an anchor. From this verb, we search to the left for the nearest noun, and consider it as the subject; we then search to the right for the nearest noun until the presence of the cue words, and consider it as the object. The detected subject, verb, and object form the cause event. In most cases, the experiencer is covertly expressed. It is, however, difficult to detect such causes in practice as causes may contain no verbs, and the two arguments are optional. Therefore, we take the clause instead of the structure of (N)+(V)+(N) as the actual cause. Examples are given in (7) and (8). For both sentences, the clause that comes before the cue word is taken as the cause event of the emotion in question. Table 4 summarizes the generalized rules for detecting the cause events of the five primary emotions in Chinese. We identify two sets of rules: 1) the specific rules that apply to all emotional instances (i.e. rules 1-13); 2) the general rules that apply to the emotional instances in which causes are not found after applying the specific set of rules (i.e. rules 14 and 15). ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh after I in F iii) C = the nearest (N)",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 191,
"end": 201,
"text": "(Lee 2010)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF10"
}
],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 2477,
"end": 2484,
"text": "Table 4",
"ref_id": "TABREF5"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "+(V)+(N) before I in F/B 2 i) E(B/F) + II/IV/V/VI(B/F) + C(B/F) + K(F) ii) E=the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before II/IV/V/VI in B/F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) before K in F i) II/IV/V/VI (B) + C(B) + E(F) + K(F)",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after II/IV/V/VI in B 4 i) E(B/F) + K(F) + IV/VII(F) + C(F/A) ii) E = a: the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F; b: the first Na/Nb/Nc/Nh in B iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after IV/VII in F/A 5 i) E(F)+K(F)+VI(A)+C(A) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after VI in A 6 i) I(F) + E(F) + K(F) + C(F/A) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh after I in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after K in F or A 7 i) E(B/F) + yue4 C yue4 K \"the more C the more K\" (F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before the first yue4 in B/F iii) C = the V in between the two yue4's in F 8",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "i) E(F) + K(F) + C(F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after K in F 9 i) E(F) + IV(F) + K(F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before IV in F iii) C = IV+(an aspectual marker) in F 10 i) K(F) + E(F) + de \"possession\"(F) + C (F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh after K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+V+(N)+\u7684+N after de in F 11 i) C(F) + K(F) + E(F)",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 268,
"end": 271,
"text": "(F)",
"ref_id": null
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh after K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) before K in F 12 i) E(B) + K(B) + III (B) + C (F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after III in F 13 i) III(B) + C(B) + E(F) + K(F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) after III in B 14 i) C(B) + E(F) + K (F) ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh before K in F iii) C = the nearest (N)",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 124,
"end": 127,
"text": "(F)",
"ref_id": null
},
{
"start": 372,
"end": 375,
"text": "(F)",
"ref_id": null
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "+(V)+(N) before K in B 15 i) E(B) +C(B) + K(F)",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "ii) E = the first Na/Nb/Nc/Nh in B iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) before K in B",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "Constraints are set to each rule to filter out incorrect causes. For instances, in Rule 1, the emotion keyword cannot be followed by the words de \"possession\"/ deshi4 \"is that\"/ shi4 \"is\" since it is very likely to have the cause event occurring after such words; in Rule 2, the cue word in III yuo3 \"to exist\" is excluded as it causes noises; whereas for Rule 4, it only applies to instances containing keywords of happiness, fear, and surprise.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"sec_num": "4.2"
},
{
"text": "An evaluation scheme is designed to assess the ability to extract the cause of an emotion in context. We specifically look into two phases of the performance of such a cause recognition system. Phase 1 assesses the detection of an emotion cooccurrence with a cause; Phrase 2 evaluates the recognition of the cause texts for an emotion.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Evaluation Metrics",
"sec_num": "5.1"
},
{
"text": "The definitions of related metrics are presented in Figure 2 . For each emotion in a sentence, if neither the gold-standard file nor the system file has a cause, both precision and recall score 1; otherwise, precision and recall are calculated by the scoring method ScoreForTwoListOfCauses. As an emotion may have more than one cause, ScoreForTwoLis-tOfCauses calculates the overlap scores between two lists of cause texts. Since emotion cause recognition is rather complicated, two relaxed string match scoring methods are selected to compare two cause texts, ScoreForTwoStrings: Relaxed Match 1 uses the minimal overlap between the gold-standard cause and the system cause. The system cause is considered as correct provided that there is at least one overlapping Chinese character; Relaxed Match 2 is more rigid which takes into account the overlap text length during scoring.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 52,
"end": 60,
"text": "Figure 2",
"ref_id": "FIGREF4"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Overall Evaluation:",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "The detection of cause occurrence is considered a preliminary task for emotion cause recognition and is compounded by the fact that neutral sentences are difficult to detect, as observed in Tokuhisa et al. (2008) . For Phase 1, each emotion keyword in a sentence has a binary tag: Y (i.e. with a cause) or N (without a cause). Similar to other NLP tasks, we adopt the common evaluation metrics, i.e. accuracy, precision, recall, and F score.",
"cite_spans": [
{
"start": 190,
"end": 212,
"text": "Tokuhisa et al. (2008)",
"ref_id": "BIBREF21"
}
],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Phase 1: The Detection of Cause Occurrence",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "The evaluation in Phase 2 is limited to the emotion keywords with a cause either in the gold-standard file or in the system file. The performance is calculated as in Overall Evaluation scheme.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Phase 2: The Detection of Causes",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "We use 80% sentences as the development data, and 20% as the test data. The baseline is designed as follows: find a verb to the left of the keyword in question, and consider the clause containing the verb as a cause. Table 5 shows the performances of the overall evaluation. We find that the overall performances of our system have significantly improved using Relaxed Match 1 and Relaxed Match 2 by 19% and 19% respectively. Although the overall performance of our system (47.95% F-score for Relaxed Match 1 and 41.67% for Relaxed Match 2) is not yet very high, it marks a good start for emotion Overall evaluation formula:",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 217,
"end": 224,
"text": "Table 5",
"ref_id": "TABREF7"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Results and Discussion",
"sec_num": "5.2"
},
{
"text": "Precision (GF, SF) = ScoreForTwoListOfCauses ( , ) 1 j j j j i i i i S GF em S S SF em S SCList GCList \u2208 \u2208 \u2208 \u2208 \u2211 \u2211 \u2211 \u2211 Recall (GF, SF) = ScoreForTwoListOfCauses ( , ) 1 j j j j i i i i S GF em S S GF em S SCList GCList \u2208 \u2208 \u2208 \u2208 \u2211 \u2211 \u2211 \u2211",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Results and Discussion",
"sec_num": "5.2"
},
{
"text": "Where GF and SF are the gold-standard cause file and system cause file respectively, and both files include the same sentences. S i is a sentence, and em j is an emotion keyword in S i . GCList j and SCList j are the lists of the gold-standard causes and system causes respectively for the emotion keyword em j .",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Results and Discussion",
"sec_num": "5.2"
},
{
"text": "If there is no cause in either GCList or SCList: Precision = 1; Recall = 1 Else:",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "ScoreForTwoListOfCauses (GCList, SCList):",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "Precision = ( , ) | | i j GCi GCList SCj SCList Max ScoreTwoStrings GC SC SCList \u2208 \u2208 \u2211 Recall = ( , ) | | i j SCj SCList GCi GCList Max ScoreTwoStrings GC SC GCList \u2208 \u2208 \u2211 ScoreForTwoStrings(GC, SC):",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "ScoreForTwoListOfCauses (GCList, SCList):",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "GC is a gold-standard cause text, and SC is a system cause text.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "ScoreForTwoListOfCauses (GCList, SCList):",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "Relaxed Match 1: If overlap existing, both precision and recall are 1; Else, both are 0. cause detection and extraction. Table 6 and 7 show the performances of the baseline and our rule-based system in Phase 1. Table 6 shows the overall accuracy, and Table 7 shows the detailed performances. In Table 6 , we find that our system and the baseline have similar high accuracy scores. Yet Table 7 shows that both systems achieve a high performance for emotions with a cause, but much worse for emotions without a cause. It is important to note that even though the naive baseline system has comparably high performance with our rule-based system in judging whether there is a cause in context, this result is biased by two facts. First, as the corpus contains more than 80% of sentences with emotion, a system which is biased toward detecting a cause, such as the baseline system, naturally performs well. In addition, once the actual cause is examined, we can see that the baseline actually detects a lot of false positives in the sense that the cause it identifies is only correct in 4.39%. Our rule-based system shows great promise in being able to deal with neutral sentences effectively and being able to detect the correct cause at least three times more often than the baseline. Table 8 shows the performances in Phase 2. Comparing to the baseline, we find that our rules improve the performance of cause recognition using Relaxed Match 1 and 2 scoring by 21% and 21% respectively. On the one hand, the 7% gap in F-score between Relaxed Match 1 and 2 also indicates that our rules can effectively locate the clause of a cause. On the other hand, the rather low performances of the baseline show that most causes recognized by the baseline are wrong although the baseline effectively detects the cause occurrence, as indicated in Table 7 . In addition, we check the accuracy (precision) and contribution (recall) of each rule. In descending order, the top four accurate rules are: Rules 7, 10, 11, and 1; and the top four contributive rules are: Rules 2, 15, 14, and 3.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [
{
"start": 121,
"end": 128,
"text": "Table 6",
"ref_id": "TABREF10"
},
{
"start": 211,
"end": 218,
"text": "Table 6",
"ref_id": "TABREF10"
},
{
"start": 251,
"end": 258,
"text": "Table 7",
"ref_id": "TABREF8"
},
{
"start": 295,
"end": 302,
"text": "Table 6",
"ref_id": "TABREF10"
},
{
"start": 385,
"end": 392,
"text": "Table 7",
"ref_id": "TABREF8"
},
{
"start": 1282,
"end": 1289,
"text": "Table 8",
"ref_id": "TABREF9"
},
{
"start": 1832,
"end": 1839,
"text": "Table 7",
"ref_id": "TABREF8"
}
],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "ScoreForTwoListOfCauses (GCList, SCList):",
"sec_num": null
},
{
"text": "Emotion processing has been a great challenge in NLP. Given the fact that an emotion is often triggered by cause events and that cause events are integral parts of emotion, we propose a linguisticdriven rule-based system for emotion cause detection, which is proven to be effective. In particular, we construct a Chinese emotion cause corpus annotated with emotions and the corresponding cause events. Since manual detection of cause events is labor-intensive and time-consuming, we intend to use the emotion cause corpus to produce automatic extraction system for emotion cause events with machine learning methods. We believe that our rule-based system is useful for many real world applications. For instance, the information regarding causal relations of emotions is important for product design, political evaluation, etc. Such a system also shed light on emotion processing as the detected emotion cause events can serve as clues for the identification of implicit emotions.",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "Conclusion",
"sec_num": "6"
},
{
"text": "http://dbo.sinica.edu.tw/SinicaCorpus/",
"cite_spans": [],
"ref_spans": [],
"eq_spans": [],
"section": "",
"sec_num": null
}
],
"back_matter": [],
"bib_entries": {
"BIBREF0": {
"ref_id": "b0",
"title": "Emotions from Text: Machine Learning for Text-based Emotion Prediction",
"authors": [
{
"first": "C",
"middle": [
"O"
],
"last": "Alm",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "D",
"middle": [],
"last": "Roth",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Sproat",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2005,
"venue": "Proceedings of the Human Language Technology Conference and the 2005 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "579--586",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Alm, C. O., D. Roth and R. Sproat. 2005. Emotions from Text: Machine Learning for Text-based Emo- tion Prediction. In Proceedings of the Human Lan- guage Technology Conference and the 2005 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Lan- guage Processing, Vancouver, Canada, 6-8 October, pp. 579-586.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF1": {
"ref_id": "b1",
"title": "Affect in Text and Speech",
"authors": [
{
"first": "C",
"middle": [
"O"
],
"last": "Alm",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2009,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Alm, C. O. 2009. Affect in Text and Speech. VDM Verlag: Saarbr\u00fccken.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF2": {
"ref_id": "b2",
"title": "A Cognitive-based Annotation System for Emotion Computing",
"authors": [
{
"first": "Y",
"middle": [],
"last": "Chen",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "S",
"middle": [
"Y M"
],
"last": "Lee",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "C.-R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Huang",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2009,
"venue": "Proceedings of the Third Linguistic Annotation Workshop",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Chen, Y., S. Y. M. Lee and C.-R. Huang. 2009. A Cog- nitive-based Annotation System for Emotion Com- puting. In Proceedings of the Third Linguistic Annotation Workshop (The LAW III), ACL 2009.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF3": {
"ref_id": "b3",
"title": "A Knowledgebased System for Headline Sentiment Tagging",
"authors": [
{
"first": "F.-R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Chaumartin",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2007,
"venue": "Proceedings of the 4th International Workshop on Semantic Evaluations",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Chaumartin, F.-R. 2007. A Knowledgebased System for Headline Sentiment Tagging. In Proceedings of the 4th International Workshop on Semantic Evalua- tions.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF4": {
"ref_id": "b4",
"title": "On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection",
"authors": [
{
"first": "C",
"middle": [],
"last": "Darwin",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": null,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Darwin, C. 1859. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection. London: John Murray.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF5": {
"ref_id": "b5",
"title": "The Passions of the Soul",
"authors": [
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Descartes",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": null,
"venue": "The Philosophical Writings of Descartes",
"volume": "1",
"issue": "",
"pages": "325--404",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Descartes, R. 1649. The Passions of the Soul. In J. Cot- tingham et al. (Eds), The Philosophical Writings of Descartes. Vol. 1, 325-404.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF6": {
"ref_id": "b6",
"title": "Expression and the Nature of Emotion",
"authors": [
{
"first": "P",
"middle": [],
"last": "Ekman",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1984,
"venue": "Approaches to Emotion",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "319--343",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Ekman, P. 1984. Expression and the Nature of Emotion. In Scherer, K. and P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to Emotion. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. 319- 343.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF7": {
"ref_id": "b7",
"title": "What is an Emotion? Mind",
"authors": [
{
"first": "W",
"middle": [],
"last": "James",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": null,
"venue": "",
"volume": "9",
"issue": "",
"pages": "188--205",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "James, W. 1884. What is an Emotion? Mind, 9(34):188- 205.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF8": {
"ref_id": "b8",
"title": "UA-ZBSA: A Headline Emotion Classification through Web Information",
"authors": [
{
"first": "Z",
"middle": [],
"last": "Kozareva",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "B",
"middle": [],
"last": "Navarro",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "S",
"middle": [],
"last": "Vazquez",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "A",
"middle": [],
"last": "Nibtoyo",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2007,
"venue": "Proceedings of the 4th International Workshop on Semantic Evaluations",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Kozareva, Z., B. Navarro, S. Vazquez, and A. Nibtoyo. 2007. UA-ZBSA: A Headline Emotion Classifica- tion through Web Information. In Proceedings of the 4th International Workshop on Semantic Evalua- tions.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF9": {
"ref_id": "b9",
"title": "Metaphor and Emotion: Language, Culture and Body in Human Feeling",
"authors": [
{
"first": "Z",
"middle": [],
"last": "K\u00f6vecses",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2000,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "K\u00f6vecses, Z. 2000. Metaphor and Emotion: Language, Culture and Body in Human Feeling. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF10": {
"ref_id": "b10",
"title": "A Linguistic Approach towards Emotion Detection and Classification",
"authors": [
{
"first": "S",
"middle": [
"Y M"
],
"last": "Lee",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2010,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Lee, S. Y. M. 2010. A Linguistic Approach towards Emotion Detection and Classification. Ph.D. Disser- tation. Hong Kong.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF11": {
"ref_id": "b11",
"title": "Emotion Cause Events: Corpus Construction and Analysis",
"authors": [
{
"first": "S",
"middle": [
"Y M"
],
"last": "Lee",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "C",
"middle": [],
"last": "Ying",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "C.-R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Huang",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2010,
"venue": "Proceedings of The Seventh International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2010)",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Lee, S. Y. M., C. Ying, and C.-R. Huang. 2010. Emo- tion Cause Events: Corpus Construction and Analy- sis. In Proceedings of The Seventh International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2010). May 19-21. Malta.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF12": {
"ref_id": "b12",
"title": "Explicit Epistemic Markup of Causes in Emotion Constructions",
"authors": [
{
"first": "S",
"middle": [
"Y M"
],
"last": "Lee",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "C.-R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Huang",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2009,
"venue": "The Fifth International Conference on Contemporary Chinese Grammar. Hong Kong",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Lee, S. Y. M. and C.-R. Huang. 2009. Explicit Epistem- ic Markup of Causes in Emotion Constructions. The Fifth International Conference on Contemporary Chinese Grammar. Hong Kong. November 27 -De- cember 1.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF13": {
"ref_id": "b13",
"title": "Emotion Sensitive News Agent: An Approach Towards User Centric Emotion Sensing from the News",
"authors": [
{
"first": "S",
"middle": [
"M"
],
"last": "Masum",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "H",
"middle": [],
"last": "Prendinger",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "M",
"middle": [],
"last": "Ishizuka",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2007,
"venue": "Proceedings of the IEEE/WIC/ACM International Conference on Web Intelligence",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Masum, S. M., H. Prendinger, and M. Ishizuka. 2007. Emotion Sensitive News Agent: An Approach To- wards User Centric Emotion Sensing from the News. In Proceedings of the IEEE/WIC/ACM International Conference on Web Intelligence.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF14": {
"ref_id": "b14",
"title": "A Corpus-based Approach to Finding Happiness",
"authors": [
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Mihalcea",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "H",
"middle": [],
"last": "Liu",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2006,
"venue": "Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium on Computational Approaches to Weblogs",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Mihalcea, R. and H. Liu. 2006. A Corpus-based Ap- proach to Finding Happiness. In Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium on Computational Ap- proaches to Weblogs.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF15": {
"ref_id": "b15",
"title": "The Cognitive Structure of Emotions",
"authors": [
{
"first": "A",
"middle": [],
"last": "Ortony",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "G",
"middle": [
"L"
],
"last": "Clone",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "A",
"middle": [],
"last": "Collins",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1988,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Ortony A., G. L. Clone, and A. Collins. 1988. The Cog- nitive Structure of Emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF16": {
"ref_id": "b16",
"title": "Affective Computing",
"authors": [
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [
"W"
],
"last": "Picard",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1995,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Picard, R.W. 1995. Affective Computing. Cambridge. MA: The MIT Press.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF17": {
"ref_id": "b17",
"title": "Emotions: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis",
"authors": [
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Plutchik",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1980,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Plutchik, R. 1980. Emotions: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF18": {
"ref_id": "b18",
"title": "TimeML Annotation Guidelines",
"authors": [
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Saur\u00ed",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "J",
"middle": [],
"last": "Littman",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Knippen",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Gaizauskas",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "A",
"middle": [],
"last": "Setzer",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "J",
"middle": [],
"last": "Pustejovsky",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2004,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Saur\u00ed, R., J. Littman, R. Knippen, R. Gaizauskas, A. Setzer, and J. Pustejovsky. 2004. TimeML Annota- tion Guidelines. http://www.timeml.org.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF19": {
"ref_id": "b19",
"title": "The Collected Works of Spinoza",
"authors": [
{
"first": "B",
"middle": [],
"last": "Spinoza",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1985,
"venue": "Ethics. In E. Curley",
"volume": "1",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Spinoza, B. 1985. Ethics. In E. Curley, The Collected Works of Spinoza. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton Univer- sity Press. Vol 1.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF20": {
"ref_id": "b20",
"title": "Toward a Cognitive Semantics",
"authors": [
{
"first": "L",
"middle": [],
"last": "Talmy",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2000,
"venue": "",
"volume": "1",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics. Vol. 1and 2. Cambridge: MIT Press.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF21": {
"ref_id": "b21",
"title": "Emotion Classification Using Massive Examples Extracted from the Web",
"authors": [
{
"first": "R",
"middle": [],
"last": "Tokuhisa",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "K",
"middle": [],
"last": "Inui",
"suffix": ""
},
{
"first": "Y",
"middle": [],
"last": "Matsumoto",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2008,
"venue": "Proceedings of COLING",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Tokuhisa, R., K. Inui, and Y. Matsumoto. 2008. Emo- tion Classification Using Massive Examples Ex- tracted from the Web. In Proceedings of COLING.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF22": {
"ref_id": "b22",
"title": "The Evolution of Emotions in Humans: A Darwinian-Durkheimian Analysis",
"authors": [
{
"first": "J",
"middle": [
"H"
],
"last": "Turner",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1996,
"venue": "Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour",
"volume": "26",
"issue": "",
"pages": "1--34",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Turner, J. H. 1996. The Evolution of Emotions in Hu- mans: A Darwinian-Durkheimian Analysis. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 26:1-34.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF23": {
"ref_id": "b23",
"title": "On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect",
"authors": [
{
"first": "J",
"middle": [
"H"
],
"last": "Turner",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 2000,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Turner, J. H. 2000. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. California: Stanford University Press.",
"links": null
},
"BIBREF24": {
"ref_id": "b24",
"title": "Emotions Across Languages and Cultures: Diversity and Universals",
"authors": [
{
"first": "A",
"middle": [],
"last": "Wierzbicka",
"suffix": ""
}
],
"year": 1999,
"venue": "",
"volume": "",
"issue": "",
"pages": "",
"other_ids": {},
"num": null,
"urls": [],
"raw_text": "Wierzbicka, A. 1999. Emotions Across Languages and Cultures: Diversity and Universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.",
"links": null
}
},
"ref_entries": {
"FIGREF0": {
"type_str": "figure",
"text": "-3.SG.M dui4-for zhe4-DET ge4-CL chong1man3full of nong2hou4-dense ai4yi4-love de-DE xiang3fa3idea gao1xing4-happy de-DE shou3wu3zu2dao3-flourish.",
"uris": null,
"num": null
},
"FIGREF1": {
"type_str": "figure",
"text": "An Example of Cause Event Annotation",
"uris": null,
"num": null
},
"FIGREF2": {
"type_str": "figure",
"text": "Rule 1: i) C(B/F) + I(F) + E(F) + K(F)ii) E = the nearest Na/Nb/Nc/Nh after I in F iii) C = the nearest (N)+(V)+(N) before I in F/B",
"uris": null,
"num": null
},
"FIGREF3": {
"type_str": "figure",
"text": "1 i) C(B/F) + I(F) + E(F) + K(F)",
"uris": null,
"num": null
},
"FIGREF4": {
"type_str": "figure",
"text": "The Definitions of Metrics for Cause Detection",
"uris": null,
"num": null
},
"TABREF1": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table><tr><td>Emotions</td><td>Extracted</td><td>No. of Instances Emotional</td><td>With Causes</td></tr><tr><td>Happiness</td><td>1,644</td><td>1,327</td><td>1,132 (85%)</td></tr><tr><td>Sadness</td><td>901</td><td>616</td><td>468 (76%)</td></tr><tr><td>Fear</td><td>897</td><td>689</td><td>567 (82%)</td></tr><tr><td>Anger</td><td>1,175</td><td>847</td><td>629 (74%)</td></tr><tr><td>Surprise</td><td>1,341</td><td>781</td><td>664 (85%)</td></tr><tr><td>Total</td><td>5,958</td><td>4,260 (72%)</td><td>3,460 (81%)</td></tr></table>",
"text": "Summary of Corpus Data",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF2": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table><tr><td>Emotions</td><td colspan=\"2\">Cause Type (%)</td><td colspan=\"2\">Cause Position (%)</td></tr><tr><td/><td>Event</td><td>Nominal</td><td>Left</td><td>Right</td></tr><tr><td>Happiness</td><td>76</td><td>6</td><td>74</td><td>29</td></tr><tr><td>Sadness</td><td>67</td><td>8</td><td>80</td><td>20</td></tr><tr><td>Fear</td><td>68</td><td>13</td><td>52</td><td>48</td></tr><tr><td>Anger</td><td>55</td><td>18</td><td>71</td><td>26</td></tr><tr><td>Surprise</td><td>73</td><td>12</td><td>59</td><td>41</td></tr></table>",
"text": "Cause Event Position of Each Emotion",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF3": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table><tr><td>Group</td><td>Cue Words</td></tr><tr><td>I</td><td>'to cause': rang4, ling4, shi3</td></tr><tr><td>II</td><td>'to think about': e.g. xiang3 dao4, xiang3 qi3, yi1</td></tr><tr><td/><td>xiang3</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'to talk about': e.g. shuo1dao4, jiang3dao4, tan2dao4</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">III 'to say': e.g. shuo1, dao4</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">IV 'to see': e.g. kan4dao4, kan4jian4, jian4dao4</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'to hear': e.g. ting1dao4, ting1 shuo1</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'to know': e.g. zhi1dao4, de2zhi1, fa1xian4</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'to exist': you3</td></tr><tr><td>V</td><td>'for' as in 'I will do this for you': wei4, wei4le</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'for' as in 'He is too old for the job': dui4, dui4yu2</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">VI 'because': yin1, yin1wei4, you2yu2</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">VII 'is': deshi4</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'at': yu2</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'can': neng2</td></tr></table>",
"text": "Seven Groups of Linguistic Cues",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF5": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table/>",
"text": "Linguistic Rules for Cause Detection",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF7": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table><tr><td/><td/><td>Baseline</td><td/><td/><td>Rule-based System</td><td/></tr><tr><td>Emotions</td><td>Precision</td><td>Recall</td><td>F-score</td><td>Precision</td><td>Recall</td><td>F-score</td></tr><tr><td>With causes</td><td>99.42</td><td>79.74</td><td>88.50</td><td>96.871</td><td>80.851</td><td>88.139</td></tr><tr><td>Without causes</td><td>4.39</td><td>66.67</td><td>8.23</td><td>13.158</td><td>52.632</td><td>21.053</td></tr></table>",
"text": "The Overall Performances",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF8": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table><tr><td/><td/><td>Relaxed Match 1</td><td/><td/><td>Relaxed Match 2</td><td/></tr><tr><td/><td>Precision</td><td>Recall</td><td>F-score</td><td>Precision</td><td>Recall</td><td>F-score</td></tr><tr><td>Baseline</td><td>25.37</td><td>39.28</td><td>30.83</td><td>17.09</td><td>36.29</td><td>23.24</td></tr><tr><td>Our System</td><td>44.64</td><td>61.30</td><td>51.66</td><td>39.18</td><td>51.68</td><td>44.57</td></tr></table>",
"text": "The Detailed Performances in Phase 1",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF9": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table><tr><td/><td>Baseline</td><td>Rule-based System</td></tr><tr><td>Accuracy</td><td>79.56</td><td>79.38</td></tr></table>",
"text": "The Detailed Performances in Phase 2",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
},
"TABREF10": {
"html": null,
"content": "<table/>",
"text": "",
"num": null,
"type_str": "table"
}
}
}
}